HomeMy WebLinkAbout11/9/2021 Item 4a, O'Hara
Delgado, Adriana
From:Christie O'Hara <oharacr@gmail.com>
Sent:Tuesday, November
To:E-mail Council Website
Subject:11/9/2021 Item 4a
Attachments:Bratman nature experience.pdf
This message is from an External Source. Use caution when deciding to open attachments, click links, or respond.
Dear Mayor and City Council,
Thank you for all your time and staff time that has been devoted to this hot topic. I appreciate that you want to get all
the facts straight and understand the research and opinions before you make a decision. I especially applaud your
efforts to contact our native tribes in the area as I do believe that is important. I hope more effort was made to reach
out to them and to explain that the extended hours was only for an additional 2 hours during the winter on a few miles
of trails. Going through public comment I was unable to find Mona Tucker's comment that the YTT tribe was ok with the
hours as long as the animals were not distrubed (I remember this comment during the last meeting), but also there was
a letter that the Chumash were not in support. I was able to find a letter from Wendy Lucas of the Northern Chumash
Tribe in this most recent public comment and to quote her- "We speak, in indigenous ways, of learning to be in
relationship with the land. To honor its gifts that are abundant, and often times that means simply to be present and
listen. To reconnect and learn the lessons nature has to teach us. If there ever was a societal need for this reconnection,
it is now. Walking/hiking enables this- using this mountain as a race course does not."
I agree with Wendy completely in this instance. How can we reconnect with the land if we cannot have access to it? I
feel most reconnected with the land and the lessons nature has to teach us when I am on the mountain, and the
experience you feel day versus night is very different. Most of us do not use the mountain as a race course, and races
are not held on the mountain. The views of the city from the top of the mountain, the sounds and the sights, are
something that everyone should have the opportunity to experience.
In previous letters both I, CCCMB, and others had recommended setting open space open hours to the same year round
(i.e. open 6am-9pm year round). On the longest day of the year sunset is around 8:30pm which under the current
"rules" you are allowed to be on the land until 1 hour after sunset which would be 9:30pm. On the shortest day of the
year you need to be off the trails by 5:50. Even by extending hours until 8:30pm, users would still be off the trails earlier
than they are in the summer. Just because daylight hours are shorter in the winter, those who work late should not be
disadvantaged to accessing the trails. Many comments incorrectly state that there would be 24/7 access.
Safety- This is one of my other primary concerns that many are arguing about. I feel safer on the trails than running on
the sidewalk, riding on the road, or working out in a gym (germ factory, even worse with covid). As an all female council I
hope none of you have ever had to experience being catcalled while working out, but I'm sure many of you have. I've
had friends followed in a car while they were out running and attempted abduction, and I've personally have had plenty
of experiences that I'm sure you can imagine. Riding on the road has become unsafe with cars (it is said how many
deaths have occured in SLO City alone) and the trails allow an opportunity to be away from traffic, away from sounds,
and away from those who pull up to you in a car and say things that are inappropriate and harrassing. The pilot program
1
has shown that injuries and safety calls were low on the trails and if I'm correct only one call was made about a rolled
ankle. Even if it may be dark, I will always feel safer on the trails than being around cars and traffic.
Lights- I know there was a lot of concern about the lights mountain bikers use. Unfortunately there is not much research
that shows the impact on wildlife, however I would like to point out that mountain bike use overall is much lower than
hiking (as shown by the permit percentage), and mountain bikers pass by areas at a much faster rate than
hikers/runners reducing the light impact on an area of trail. The light is focused directly on the trail and when/if stopped
to take a break the light is often turned off to enjoy the dark and the views. Although a hiker may have a lower setting
on their light I would say their impact vs. light on trail could be similar to that of mountain bikers. The lights from cars
and sounds from planes overhead the mountain have a much greater impact to wildlife than the users on the trails. I
read through a lot of the Fish and Wildlife Journal that has been shared and other articles they cited, including a 300
page one by Knight in which he goes into detail how car sounds, lights, airplanes, and other surroundings (all which
include the area around Cerro San Luis) have a greater impact than those of trail users. I see way more roadkill on the
actual roads because of cars, I've never seen any "roadkill" on the trails.
False Information- I would like to request Jan Marx publicly apologize during the meeting in regards to her comments on
this topic the last city council meeting it was discussed and during others. She has provided many false statements which
has riled up her base and continued to circulate false information. With all the fake news and media these days it is
important that a public leader be truthful, honest, and respect public comment even if she does not agree with it. She
has compared those not agreeing with her to the January 6th capitol insurrectionists. This is not only vile, but
completely disrespectful and insulting. Many of us are community members who have volunteered with the city for
numerous non-profits over the years. I've had a relative that has died in the line of duty and to compare me and others
to the same group that killed other officers is out of line. She also made fun of a 10 year old boy in a photo (wasting
paper mind you to attempt to show the photo on zoom). She incorrectly stated that there are "endangered animals" on
Cerro San Luis. Staff reports never stated this. She also made false statements regarding Clint Francis, PhD and Biological
Science Professor. She has also insulted other Cal Poly professors during the 3/16/2021 meeting on this topic as well
stating "This is a small group of people, a lot of these people are Cal Poly professors and they can just go hike Poly
Canyon." Are those professors not equally important citizens of SLO because they work at Poly, and why are they being
asked to hike elsewhere when many would like to get off campus during their non-work hours. I hold all City Council
members in high regard and expect better. Inside of dividing the community please treat all public comments equally
and don't put down a differing opinion from you. I thank all those city council members that have taken the time to read
all these comments and consider both sides. City Staff has worked hard and many of us have sent in comments in
support of the city staff recommendation and to be put down and insulted is horrible.
Research- We can all pull quotes from articles and cite non-peer reviewed articles and you can always google both sides
of an argument. The Fish and Wildlife journal is of course biased toward Wildlife. I appreciate and respect all our wildlife,
we all do, but balance and compromise must happen. I attached an article by Bratman, 2015 in which "We show in
healthy participants that a brief nature experience, a 90-min walk in a natural setting, decreases both self reported
rumination and neural activity in the subgenual prefrontal cortex (sgPFC), whereas a 90-min walk in an urban
setting has no such effects on self-reported rumination or neural activity. In other studies, the sgPFC has been
associated with a self-focused behavioral withdrawal linked to rumination in both depressed and healthy individuals.
This study reveals a pathway by which nature experience may improve mental well-being and suggests that accessible
natural areas within urban contexts may be a critical resource for mental health in our rapidly urbanizing world."
In closing, if we cannot have access until 8:30pm during the winter months to one city open space, what would be your
solution? For those that will ride/hike/run anyway- do you prefer they drive to MDO or National Forest where the
wildlife corridor is even more vast and important? Or have them use other open space? The original proposal was to
2
change the hours of all SLO City open space, and after 3+ years I believe city staff has found the best solution and
compromise: One trail system in city open space that is the least impacted, limited users (65), and only until 8:30pm.
Thank you again for your time and considering all "sides" and what is best for the residents of San Luis Obispo.
Sincerely,
Christie O'Hara
3
Nature experience reduces rumination and subgenual
prefrontal cortex activation
Gregory N. Bratman
a,1, J. Paul Hamilton
b, Kevin S. Hahn
c, Gretchen C. Daily
d,e,1, and James J. Gross
c
aEmmett Interdisciplinary Program in Environment and Resources, Stanford University, Stanford, CA 94305;
bLaureate Institute for Brain Research, School of
Community Medicine, Tulsa, OK 74136;
cDepartment of Psychology, Stanford University, Stanford, CA 94305;
dCenter for Conservation Biology, Department
of Biology, and Woods Institute, Stanford University, Stanford, CA 94305; and
eGlobal Economic Dynamics and the Biosphere, Royal Swedish Academy of
Sciences, and Stockholm Resilience Centre, Stockholm 114 18, Sweden
Contributed by Gretchen C. Daily, May 28, 2015 (sent for review March 9, 2015; reviewed by Leslie Baxter, Elliot T. Berkman, and Andreas Meyer-Lindenberg)
Urbanization has many benefits, but it also is associated with
increased levels of mental illness, including depression. It has been
suggested that decreased nature experience may help to explain
the link between urbanization and mental illness. This suggestion
is supported by a growing body of correlational and experimental
evidence, which raises a further question: what mechanism(s) link
decreased nature experience to the development of mental illness?
One such mechanism might be the impact of nature exposure on
rumination, a maladaptive pattern of self-referential thought that is
associated with heightened risk for depression and other mental
illnesses. We show in healthy participants that a brief nature
experience, a 90-min walk in a natural setting, decreases both self-
reported rumination and neural activity in the subgenual prefrontal
cortex (sgPFC), whereas a 90-min walk in an urban setting has no
such effects on self-reported rumination or neural activity. In other
studies, the sgPFC has been associated with a self-focused behavioral
withdrawal linked to rumination in both depressed and healthy
individuals. This study reveals a pathway by which nature experience
may improve mental well-being and suggests that accessible natural
areas within urban contexts may be a critical resource for mental
health in our rapidly urbanizing world.
environmental neuroscience |nature experience |rumination |
psychological ecosystem services |emotion regulation
N ever before has such a large percentage of humanity been so
far removed from nature (1); more than 50% of people now
live in urban areas, and by 2050, this proportion will be 70% (2).
What are the potential mental health implications of this de-
mographic shift? Although urbanization has many benefits, it is
also associated with increased levels of mental illness, including
anxiety disorders and depression (3–5). Causal mechanisms for
this increased prevalence of mental illness are likely manifold
and are not well understood (6, 7).
One aspect of urbanization that has attracted research atten-
tion in recent years is a corresponding decrease in nature ex-
perience (8, 9). Using a variety of methodologies, researchers
have demonstrated affective and cognitive benefits of nature
experience, thereby contributing to an evolving understanding of
the types of psychological benefits of which humanity may be
deprived as urbanization continues. Correlational findings show
that growing up in rural vs. urban settings is associated with
lesser stress responsivity (3). A recent longitudinal study, track-
ing the well-being and mental distress of more than 10,000
people over a period of nearly two decades demonstrates a sig-
nificant positive effect of proximity to greenspace on well-being
(9). This effect traces to living location within the same in-
dividuals as they moved closer or further from greenspace. Other
correlational studies reveal that window views that include nat-
ural elements (compared with window views that do not) are
associated with superior memory, attention, and impulse in-
hibition (10), as well as greater feelings of subjective well-being
(11). These correlational findings are buttressed by experimental
findings showing, for example, that nature experience (usually in
urban greenspace) can improve memory and attention (12) and
increase positive mood (13). Experimenters also have used psy-
chophysiological methods to characterize the ways in which im-
ages and sounds of the natural environment lead to decreased
stress and negative emotion after participants have been sub-
jected to stressful stimuli (14, 15). Taken together, these and
numerous other studies provide compelling evidence that nature
experience may confer real psychological benefits.
Although this body of work is now substantial, there remains a
fundamental yet unanswered question: by what mechanism(s) might
nature experience buffer against the development of mental illness?
Onepossiblemechanism—and our focus here—is a decrease in
rumination, a maladaptive pattern of self-referential thought that is
associated with heightened risk for depression and other mental
illnesses (16–18) and with activity in the subgenual prefrontal cortex
(sgPFC) (19). The sgPFC has been shown to display increased ac-
tivity during sadness (20) and the behavioral withdrawal and neg-
ative self-reflective processes tiedtoruminationinhealthy(21)and
depressed (22–24) individuals.
Rumination is a prolonged and often maladaptive attentional
focus on the causes and consequences of emotions—most often,
negative, self-relational emotions (25). This pattern of thought
has been shown to predict the onset of depressive episodes (17),
as well as other mental disorders (26). Positive or neutral dis-
traction (vs. maladaptive distractions such as binge drinking of
alcohol) has been shown to decrease rumination (27). To be
effective in decreasing rumination, these positive or neutral
distractions must be engrossing, to maintain the shift of attention
Significance
More than 50% of people now live in urban areas. By 2050 this
proportion will be 70%. Urbanization is associated with in-
creased levels of mental illness, but it’s not yet clear why.
Through a controlled experiment, we investigated whether
nature experience would influence rumination (repetitive
thought focused on negative aspects of the self), a known risk
factor for mental illness. Participants who went on a 90-min
walk through a natural environment reported lower levels of
rumination and showed reduced neural activity in an area of
the brain linked to risk for mental illness compared with those
who walked through an urban environment. These results
suggest that accessible natural areas may be vital for mental
health in our rapidly urbanizing world.
Author contributions: G.N.B., J.P.H., and J.J.G. designed research; G.N.B. performed re-
search; G.N.B., J.P.H., K.S.H., and J.J.G. analyzed data; and G.N.B., J.P.H., K.S.H., G.C.D., and
J.J.G. wrote the paper.
Reviewers: L.B., Barrow Neurological Institute; E.T.B., University of Oregon; and A.M.-L.,
Central Institute of Mental Health, Medical Faculty Mannheim, University of Heidelberg.
The authors declare no conflict of interest.
1To whom correspondence may be addressed. Email: gbratman@stanford.edu or gdaily@
stanford.edu.
This article contains supporting information online at www.pnas.org/lookup/suppl/doi:10.
1073/pnas.1510459112/-/DCSupplemental.
www.pnas.org/cgi/doi/10.1073/pnas.1510459112 PNAS |July 14, 2015 |vol. 112 |no. 28 |8567–8572 PSYCHOLOGICAL ANDCOGNITIVE SCIENCES
onto the distracting stimuli (27). From this perspective, we aimed
to observe whether a 90-min nature experience has the potential to
decrease rumination. In addition to gathering self-report measures,
we examined brain activity in the sgPFC, an area that has been
shown to be particularly active during the type of maladaptive, self-
reflective thought and behavioral withdrawal that occurs during
rumination (19). This behavioral and neural evidence—when taken
together—would provide convincing evidence for a change in ru-
mination resultant from nature experience.
We quantified the impacts of a brief nature experience on
rumination and neural activity in the sgPFC through a controlled
experiment, comparing changes that occur in a 90-min nature
walk to those in a 90-min urban walk. We hypothesized that we
would observe decreased rumination and decreased neural ac-
tivity in the sgPFC for urban residents who experienced a nature
walk, whereas we would not observe such a decrease in those
who experienced an urban walk. We obtained measures of in-
dividuals’self-reported levels of rumination using the rumination
portion of the Reflection Rumination Questionnaire (RRQ)
(28). We documented activity in the sgPFC by using a neuro-
imaging method called arterial spin labeling (ASL, presented
more fully in Methods), through which we measured regional
cerebral blood flow (rCBF): the volume of cerebral blood pass-
ing through the region of interest. This technique can detect
effects associated with longer-lasting psychological phenomena
such as rumination, in contrast to momentary, reactive emotional
responses such as a startle response (29).
Thirty-eight healthy participants took part in the study. Al-
though rumination is often studied in the context of clinically
depressed individuals, we studied participants with no history of
mental disorder to broaden the applicability of our findings. Our
sample comprised individuals residing in urban environments. We
posited that these individuals, although currently healthy, would
enter the study with a somewhat elevated level of rumination
resulting from the ongoing and chronic stressors associated with
urban experience, and their corresponding deprivation of regular
contact with nature. We therefore hypothesized that a nature
experience would reduce the baseline rumination levels of such
participants, compared with those who had an urban experience.
On arrival at our laboratory, each participant completed a self-
report measure of rumination (RRQ) and underwent our scan-
ning procedure. We then randomly assigned each participant to
a 90-min walk in either a natural environment (19 participants)
or urban environment (19 participants). The nature walk took
place near Stanford University, in a greenspace comprising
grassland with scattered oak trees and shrubs. The urban walk
took place on the busiest thoroughfare in nearby Palo Alto (El
Camino Real), a street with three to four lanes in each direction
and a steady stream of traffic (Fig. S1). After the walk, each
participant returned to the laboratory and provided a second,
follow-up self-report of levels of rumination (RRQ) before un-
dergoing a second resting-state ASL scan. Transportation to and
from the walk was via a car ride of 15-min duration (for both
walks). Participants were given a smartphone and told to take 10
photographs during their walk (Fig. S2). These photographs
were used to verify that participants went on the walk. We also
tracked the phone itself during the walk, as further verification
that the correct route was taken by each participant.
Results
To analyze the impact of nature experience on self-reported
rumination, we conducted a two-way ANOVA, with time as a
within-subjects factor (before vs. after the walk) and environ-
ment as a between-subjects factor (nature walk vs. urban walk).
This analysis revealed an interaction between time and envi-
ronment [F(1,35)=3.51,P =0.07,η2
p =0.09]. Consistent with
our hypothesis, follow-up t tests indicated that our results were
driven by a decrease in self-reported rumination for the nature
group but not for the urban group (Fig. 1A). There was a simple
effect of time for the nature group [t(17)=−2.69,P <0.05,d =
0.34; mean change pre- to postwalk =−2.33, SE =0.55; mean
score prewalk =35.39, SE =1.60; mean score postwalk =33.06,
SE =1.61], with decreases from pre- to postwalk. There was no
such effect for the urban group (mean score prewalk =30.11,
SE =2.61; mean score postwalk =30.16, SE =2.50).
To analyze the impact of nature experience on blood perfusion
in the sgPFC, we conducted a similarly structured ANOVA with
time as a within-subjects factor (before vs. after the walk) and
environment as a between-subjects factor (nature walk vs. urban
walk). Clusters reflecting a significant time-by-environment in-
teraction were corrected for familywise error (FWE; voxelwise
P =0.05, cluster threshold =1,713 mm
3) for multiple compari-
sons across the whole brain. The sgPFC was the a priori area of
interest in this study (Table S1 and Fig. S3 for whole brain
analyses). All reported perfusion values are in units of milliliters
of blood per 100 g tissue per minute. As hypothesized, sgPFC
perfusion showed an interaction effect of time by environment,
indicating an effect of the nature walk vs. the urban walk [Fig.
1B;F(1,29)=23.41,P <0.0001,η2
p =0.45].
We investigated the composition of this interaction in the
sgPFC by examining (for each participant and scan) cerebral
blood flow estimates centered at the cluster peak. As predicted,
follow-up t tests revealed that our results were driven by de-
creases in blood flow resulting from nature vs. urban experience.
There was an effect of time for the nature group [t(15)=−6.89,
P <0.0001,d =1.01] with decreases from pre- to postwalk in the
nature group, but there was no effect of time for the urban group
(Fig. 1C).
To assess whether the effects of the nature vs. urban experi-
ence on rCBF arose from different physiological effects of these
walks (e.g., potential differences in the physical demands of each
walk), heart rate and respiration rate were measured during the
rCBF scans (physiology data for three of the participants were
eliminated because sensors were loosened while participants
were placed into the MRI scanner). Although the nature walk
contained short sections of path with a higher slope gradient
than the urban walk, and the cumulative elevation gain of the
walks did differ, with more total gain in the nature walk than in
the urban walk (Methods), total distances of the walks were
equal; we observed no interaction effect on physiology (i.e.,
differential change in heart rate or respiration rate due to en-
vironment). Heart rate showed a main effect of time [F(1,26)=
5.21,P <0.05,η2
p =0.17; mean change pre- to postwalk =5.51,
SE =2.37; mean score prewalk =68.54, SE =2.22; mean score
postwalk =74.05, SE =3.88], but no interaction effect of time by
environment [F(1,26)=0.08, not significant].Respiration rate
showed neither a main effect of time nor a time-by-environment
interaction (all P >0.37). The lack of group by time effects for
either physiological measure speaks against the possibility that
observed behavioral and brain effects were due to residual dif-
ferences in walk-related physiological activation.
Discussion
Our results indicate that nature experience reduced rumination
and sgPFC activation. Participants who went on a 90-min nature
walk showed reductions in self-reported rumination and de-
creases in sgPFC activity, whereas those who went on an urban
walk did not show these effects. Given the documented link
between rumination and risk for depression and other psycho-
logical illnesses, the reduction in rumination among those with
the nature experience suggests one possible mechanism by which
urbanization—which reduces opportunities for nature experi-
ence—may be linked to mental illness. This suggestion draws
support from our finding that at a neurobiological level, nature
experience led to decreases in sgPFC activity, a brain region that
previously has been shown to be associated with a self-focused
8568 |www.pnas.org/cgi/doi/10.1073/pnas.1510459112 Bratman et al.
behavioral withdrawal linked to rumination in both depressed
and healthy individuals.
These findings support the view that natural environments
may confer psychological benefits to humans (30). In the liter-
ature on “restorative”environments (31), researchers have shown
that individuals tend to select favorite environments as a means
to transform negative psychological states to more positive ones.
These areas tend to be natural environments, although not ex-
clusively so. Natural environments with pleasing aesthetic qual-
ities including open views (32) and lack of loud, distracting
noises are often chosen as preferred restorative environments
(30). Effects of these landscapes are captured in the Perceived
Restorativeness Scale (33), and include those that engender
somewhat effortless,“soft fascination”; the “sense of belonging”;
and the “sense of being away.”This literature relates to our
findings insofar as we may consider these preferred environ-
ments to engender the type of positive distraction that has been
shown to decrease rumination and negative affect in depressed
individuals (27). Specifically, our findings of decreased sgPFC
activity in the nature group point to a possible causal mechanism
for the affective benefits of nature experience.
Our findings may have relevance beyond the neural correlates
of rumination. Activity in the sgPFC is also more broadly tied to
behavioral withdrawal (19). Although we observed peak activity
in the sgPFC, this significant cluster of voxels also includes the
perigenual anterior cingulate cortex (pACC): a region that has
been shown to display increased reactivity in individuals born in
urban areas during social stress processing (3). Other forms of
affective appraisal, emotion regulation, and reactivity to social
hierarchies involve coordinated activity of this region with other
Fig. 1.The impact of nature experience on self-reported rumination and blood perfusion to the sgPFC. (A) Change in self-reported rumination (postwalk
minus prewalk) for participants randomly assigned to take a 90-min walk either in a natural setting or in an urban setting. (B) A time-by-environment in-
teraction in blood perfusion was evident in the sgPFC.F map of significant interactions at a threshold of P <0.05, FWE corrected for multiple comparisons.
(C ) Change in blood perfusion (postwalk minus prewalk) for participants randomly assigned to take a 90-min walk either in a natural setting or in an urban
setting. Error bars represent SE within subjects: *P <0.05, ***P <0.001.
Bratman et al.PNAS |July 14, 2015 |vol. 112 |no. 28 |8569 PSYCHOLOGICAL ANDCOGNITIVE SCIENCES
areas of the brain, including the insula, ventral striatum, and
amygdala (34). Decreased functional connectivity between the
pACC and amygdala is found in schizophrenia (3) and bipolar
disorder (35) and is a predictor of anxiety (34). Considered
without the context provided by self-reports of rumination,
sgPFC findings could be related to the neural processing of
sadness (20), guilt, remorse, negative autobiographical narra-
tives, or peer rejection (19, 36, 37). It is also possible that other
psychological processes (e.g., stress or anxiety processing) or
hormones (e.g., dopamine or oxytocin release) may mediate the
affective benefits of nature experience. These possibilities pro-
vide a rich area for further study.
Our findings of the effects of a relatively brief nature experi-
ence suggest that feasible investments in access to natural envi-
ronments could yield important benefits for the “mental capital”
(38) of cities and nations. More research is needed to refine our
understanding of the “production functions”of natural envi-
ronments (39) for mental health benefits, clarifying both key
characteristics of the environments and the duration, frequency,
and types of experience that generate benefits (40). By ac-
counting for these psychological ecosystem services (40), we can
better assess the value that natural areas provide with respect to
mental health, an essential issue given the significant contribu-
tion of depression and other mental illnesses to the global bur-
den of disease (41).
As empirical understanding builds regarding the ways in which
nature experience benefits human cognitive function and mood,
we can move toward a more complete incorporation of these
benefits into the paradigm of ecosystem services. Doing so will
require new research on the ways in which these impacts vary
with biophysical attributes of natural land- and seascapes, fre-
quency and duration of nature experience, as well as character-
istics and personality attributes of the individual. Already, some
cities and nations are incorporating these benefits into urban
design, treating proximity of buildings (especially schools) and
public access to greenspace as important aspects of city planning
that may influence stress, mental health, and even cognitive
functioning (42–46). With deeper understanding, mental health
benefits of nature can be incorporated into a wide array of ini-
tiatives and investments in sustainable cities and conservation
(47–49). Understanding the mechanisms by which nature expe-
rience buffers against the negative repercussions of urban life
(50) will help us better plan for an ever more urban world.
Methods
Ethics Statement.The study was approved by the Stanford University Human
Subjects Committee. Participants were paid $20/h to participate in the study
and signed informed consent.
Participants.Thirty-eight participants (18 female, total mean age =26.6 y)
with no current or past diagnosis of neurologic or psychiatric disorder were
invited to participate in a study that measured affective and cognitive
functioning before and after a walk. All participants lived and worked in
urban parts of the San Francisco Bay Area. No reference was made to the
type of environment they would experience during their walk. Participants
had normal or corrected-to-normal vision and were not taking any psycho-
tropic medications. Each participant was randomly assigned to either a na-
ture walk (19 participants; 8 females, mean age =25.9 y) or an urban walk
(19 participants; 10 females, mean age =27.2 y), and each underwent our
scanning procedure before and after the walk. Groups did not differ by age
[t(36)=0.48,P >0.1] or sex [χ2(1)=0.475,P >0.1]. Seven participants had to
be eliminated before perfusion analysis due to excessive movement during
scanning, leaving 31 participants (16 female, mean age =26.4 y, all right-
handed) for perfusion analysis. One participant was eliminated in analysis of
self-reported rumination due to a decrease in rumination after nature ex-
perience that was 3 SDs below the mean.
Locations and Instructions for Walks.The nature walk took place in a
greenspace near Stanford University spanning an area ∼60 m northwest of
Junipero Serra Boulevard and extending away from the street in a 5.3-km
loop, including a significant stretch that is far (>1 km) from the sounds and
sights of the surrounding residential area. As one proxy for urbanicity, we
measured the proportion of impervious surface (e.g., asphalt, buildings, side-
walks) within 50 m of the center of the walking path (Fig. S4). Ten percent of
the area within 50 m of the center of the path comprised of impervious sur-
face (primarily of the asphalt path). Cumulative elevation gain of this walk was
155 m. The natural environment of the greenspace comprises open California
grassland with scattered oaks and native shrubs, abundant birds, and occa-
sional mammals (ground squirrels and deer). Views include neighboring, scenic
hills, and distant views of the San Francisco Bay, and the southern portion of
the Bay Area (including Palo Alto and Mountain View to the south, and Menlo
Park and Atherton to the north). No automobiles, bicycles, or dogs are per-
mitted on the path through the greenspace.
The urbanwalk took place onthebusiestthoroughfareinnearby Palo Alto
(El Camino Real), a street with three to four lanes in each direction and a
steadystreamoftraffic.Participantswereinstructedtowalkdownonesideof
the street in a southeasterly direction for 2.65 km, before turning around at a
specific point marked on a map. This spot was chosen as the midpoint of the
walk for the urban walk to match the nature walk with respect to total
distance and exercise. Participants were instructed to cross the street at a
pedestrian crosswalk/stoplight, and return on the other side of the street (to
simulate theloopcomponentofthenaturewalk andgreatlyreducerepeated
encounters with the same environmental stimuli on the return portion of the
walk),foratotaldistanceof5.3km;76%oftheareawithin50mofthecenter
of this section of El Camino was comprised of impervious surfaces (of roads
and buildings) (Fig. S4). Cumulative elevation gain of this walk was 4 m. This
stretch of road consists of a significant amount of noise from passing cars.
Buildings are almost entirely single- to double-story units, primarily busi-
nesses (fast food establishments, cell phone stores, motels, etc.). Participants
were instructed to remain on the sidewalk bordering the busy street and not
to enter any buildings. Although this was the most urban area we could
select for a walk that was a similar distance from the MRI facility as the
nature walk, scattered trees were present on both sides of El Camino
Real. Thus, our effects may represent a conservative estimate of effects
of nature experience, as our urban group’s experience was not devoid of
natural elements.
For both walks, participants were transported (2 km) to the starting point
of the walk by car, individually, and went on the walk alone. They were given
a smartphone with which they were instructed to take 10 photographs of
whatever captured their attention. These instructions were given primarily to
help hide the purpose of the study, as well as to provide confirmatory evi-
dence that the participant completed the entire walk on returning to the
start/end point. We also tracked the participants during their walks through
the use of a tracking application installed on the phone, as further confir-
matory evidence that they went on the assigned walks and did not stray
from their instructed routes, stop at specific spots, or go inside of buildings.
Per our tracking data and photographic evidence, all participants completed
their walks as instructed.
Rumination.Rumination was assessed using the RRQ (28). The RRQ is divided
into two scales (rumination and reflection). In this study, only the rumination
scale was used, as this was our dependent variable of interest. This scale
consists of 12 items that measure ruminative tendencies (e.g.,“My attention
is often focused on aspects of myself I wish I’d stop thinking about”), each
rated on a five-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5
(strongly agree). Higher means of the sum of scores indicate higher degrees
of rumination.
Image Acquisition and Reduction.Scans were acquired using a 3-T General
Electric MR750 Discovery Scanner at the Stanford Center for Cognitive and
Neurobiological Imaging. The high-resolution T1-weighted MR images in-
cluded 186 0.9-mm slices with an in-plane resolution of 0.898 mm
2. High-
resolution image acquisition was followed by a pulsed continuous ASL flow
alternating inversion recovery (FAIR) sequence using a postlabel delay of
1,525 ms; TR =4.674 s; TE =10.968 ms; FOV =240 × 240 mm; matrix size =
512 × 8; 38 axial slices; slice thickness =3.2 mm; voxel dimensions 1.875 ×
1.875 × 3.2 mm, one total measurement, for a total acquisition time of 4 min
and 31 s.
We acquired perfusion weighted data and proton density maps and then
combined information from those per the standard CBF flow equation
quantification algorithm (51)
8570 |www.pnas.org/cgi/doi/10.1073/pnas.1510459112 Bratman et al.
CBF =6,000 × λ
1 −exp −ST ðsÞ
T1t ðsÞ exp PLDðsÞ
T1b ðsÞ
2T1b ðsÞ 1 −exp −LT ðsÞ
T1b ðsÞ E × NEXPW
PW
SFpw PD ,
where T1b is T1 of blood and is assumed to be 1.6 s at 3 T. Partial saturation
of the reference image (PD) is corrected for by using a T1t of 1.2 s (typical of
gray matter). ST is saturation time (set to 2 s).λ is the partial coefficient that
is set to the whole brain average (0.9).E is overall efficiency (0.6), a com-
bination of inversion efficiency (0.8) and background suppression efficiency
(0.75).PLD is postlabeling delay used for the ASL sequence, and LT is the
labeling duration (1. 5 s).PW is perfusion weighted (or raw difference) im-
age.SPPW is the scaling factor of the PW sequence.NEXPW is the number
excitation for PW images. A 500-μs Hanning pulse was used for the labeling
pulse, and the labeling gradient during the pulse is 0.7 G/cm, with an av-
erage gradient of 0.07 G/cm. We used a 2-s saturation time for the reference
image, which is a PD (and T1) weighted saturation recovery image. Back-
ground suppression was used with five inversion pulses.
This calculation rendered voxelwise quantitative maps reflecting milliliters
of blood per 100 g tissue per minute (volume × time/mass). We coregistered
ASL volumes to each individual’s anatomical scans and then performed a
combined affine and nonlinear warping process of the anatomical data to
standard (Talairach) space, using AFNI’s 3dQWarp. We then resampled the
ASL data to a 1-mm
3 voxel dimension. Following this, we applied the same
warping parameters to the ASL data. Finally, to account for potential gen-
eral changes in cerebral blood flow resulting from the distinct walks, we
mean-normalized each participant’s regional CBF estimates relative to the
mean CBF in gray matter. The same protocol was followed before and after
each walk for every participant.
Our dependent variable was blood perfusion, measured as milliliters of
blood per 100 g tissue per minute (volume × time/mass). We implemented an
investigation of interaction of time-by-environment effects using AFNI’s
3dMVM (52). We then used t tests to further analyze data from our region of
interest (sgPFC) that showed this interaction to better understand the
composition of these effects.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS.We thank P. Kareiva and H. Tallis for comments on
the manuscript and L. Bugatus, S. Kolarik, N. Le, B. Levy, S. Maples, S. McClure,
C. Chambliss-Rudiger, J. Ryan, C. Shin, A. Swenson, C. Tan, M. Wibowo, and
G. Young for research assistance. We also thank P. R. Ehrlich for many helpful
discussions. We are grateful to members of the Stanford Center for Conservation
Biology, the Stanford Psychophysiology Laboratory, and the Emmett Interdisci-
plinary Program in Environment and Resources (EIPER), and for funding from the
Winslow Foundation, the George Rudolf Fellowship Fund, the Victoria and
David Rogers Fund, the Mr. & Mrs. Dean A. McGee Fund, the Stanford Center
for Cognitive and Neurobiological Imaging, and EIPER. G.N.B. was supported
by the Stanford Graduate Fellowship Program in Science and Engineering (as
a David and Lucille Packard Fellow) and the Stanford Interdisciplinary Grad-
uate Fellowship Program (as a James and Nancy Kelso Fellow).
1. Turner WR, Nakamura T, Dinetti M (2004) Global urbanization and the separation of
humans from nature.Bioscience 54(6):585–590.
2. Dye C (2008) Health and urban living.Science 319(5864):766–769.
3. Lederbogen F, et al. (2011) City living and urban upbringing affect neural social stress
processing in humans.Nature 474(7352):498–501.
4. Peen J, Schoevers RA, Beekman AT, Dekker J (2010) The current status of urban-rural
differences in psychiatric disorders.Acta Psychiatr Scand 121(2):84–93.
5. Wang JL (2004) Rural-urban differences in the prevalence of major depression and
associated impairment.Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol 39(1):19–25.
6. Goodwin RD, Taha F (2014) Global health benefits of being raised in a rural setting:
Results from the National Comorbidity Survey.Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 68(6):395–403.
7. Lee J, Park B-J, Ohira T, Kagawa T, Miyazaki Y (2015) Acute effects of exposure to a
traditional rural environment on urban dwellers: A crossover field study in terraced
farmland.Int J Environ Res Public Health 12(2):1874–1893.
8. Maller C, Townsend M, Pryor A, Brown P, St Leger L (2006) Healthy nature healthy
people:‘Contact with nature’as an upstream health promotion intervention for
populations.Health Promot Int 21(1):45–54.
9. White MP, Alcock I, Wheeler BW, Depledge MH (2013) Would you be happier living in
a greener urban area? A fixed-effects analysis of panel data.Psychol Sci 24(6):
920–928.
10. Taylor AF, Kuo FE, Sullivan WC (2002) Views of nature and self-discipline: evidence
from inner city children.J Environ Psychol 22(1-2):49–63.
11. Kaplan R (2001) The nature of the view from home.Environ Behav 33(4):507–542.
12. Berman MG, et al. (2012) Interacting with nature improves cognition and affect for
individuals with depression.J Affect Disord 140(3):300–305.
13. Hartig T, Mitchell R, de Vries S, Frumkin H (2014) Nature and health.Annu Rev Public
Health 35:207–228.
14. Ulrich R, et al. (1991) Stress recovery during exposure to natural and urban envi-
ronments.J Environ Psychol 11(3):201–230.
15. Brown DK, Barton JL, Gladwell VF (2013) Viewing nature scenes positively affects
recovery of autonomic function following acute-mental stress.Environ Sci Technol
47(11):5562–5569.
16. Nolen-Hoeksema S, Morrow J (1993) Effects of rumination and distraction on natu-
rally occurring depressed mood.Cogn Emotion 7(6):561–570.
17. Nolen-Hoeksema S (2000) The role of rumination in depressive disorders and mixed
anxiety/depressive symptoms.J Abnorm Psychol 109(3):504–511.
18. Kuehner C, Weber I (1999) Responses to depression in unipolar depressed patients:
An investigation of Nolen-Hoeksema’s response styles theory.Psychol Med 29(6):
1323–1333.
19. Hamilton JP, Farmer M, Fogelman P, Gotlib IH (2015) Depressive rumination, the
default-mode network, and the dark matter of clinical neuroscience [published online
February 24, 2015].Biol Psychiatry, 10.1016/j.biopsych.2015.02.020.
20. Mayberg HS, et al. (1999) Reciprocal limbic-cortical function and negative mood:
Converging PET findings in depression and normal sadness.Am J Psychiatry 156(5):
675–682.
21. Kross E, Davidson M, Weber J, Ochsner K (2009) Coping with emotions past: The
neural bases of regulating affect associated with negative autobiographical memo-
ries.Biol Psychiatry 65(5):361–366.
22. Berman MG, et al. (2011) Depression, rumination and the default network.Soc Cogn
Affect Neurosci 6(5):548–555.
23. Hamilton JP, Glover GH, Hsu J-J, Johnson RF, Gotlib IH (2011) Modulation of sub-
genual anterior cingulate cortex activity with real-time neurofeedback.Hum Brain
Mapp 32(1):22–31.
24. Krishnan V, Nestler EJ (2008) The molecular neurobiology of depression.Nature
455(7215):894–902.
25. Nolen-Hoeksema S (1991) Responses to depression and their effects on the duration
of depressive episodes.J Abnorm Psychol 100(4):569–582.
26. Ghaznavi S, Deckersbach T (2012) Rumination in bipolar disorder: Evidence for an
unquiet mind.Biol Mood Anxiety Disord 2(2):1–11.
27. Nolen-Hoeksema S, Wisco BE, Lyubomirsky S (2008) Rethinking rumination.Perspect
Psychol Sci 3(5):400–424.
28. Trapnell PD, Campbell JD (1999) Private self-consciousness and the five-factor model
of personality: Distinguishing rumination from reflection.J Pers Soc Psychol 76(2):
284–304.
29. Detre JA, Rao H, Wang DJJ, Chen YF, Wang Z (2012) Applications of arterial spin
labeled MRI in the brain.J Magn Reson Imaging 35(5):1026–1037.
30. Korpela KM, Hartig T, Kaiser FG, Fuhrer U (2001) Restorative experience and self-
regulation in favorite places.Environ Behav 33(4):572–589.
31. Hartig T, Staats H (2003) Guest editors’introduction: Restorative environments.
J Environ Psychol 23(2):103–107.
32. Herzog TR (2003) Assessing the restorative components of environments.J Environ
Psychol 23(2):159–170.
33. Hartig T, Korpela K, Evans GW, Gärling T (1997) A measure of restorative quality in
environments.Housing.Theory Soc 14(4):175–194.
34. Pezawas L, et al. (2005) 5-HTTLPR polymorphism impacts human cingulate-amygdala
interactions: A genetic susceptibility mechanism for depression.Nat Neurosci 8(6):
828–834.
35. Wang F, et al. (2009) Functional and structural connectivity between the perigenual
anterior cingulate and amygdala in bipolar disorder.Biol Psychiatry 66(5):516–521.
36. Zahn R, et al. (2009) The neural basis of human social values: Evidence from functional
MRI.Cereb Cortex 19(2):276–283.
37. Masten CL, et al. (2011) Subgenual anterior cingulate responses to peer rejection: A
marker of adolescents’risk for depression.Dev Psychopathol 23(1):283–292.
38. Beddington J, et al. (2008) The mental wealth of nations.Nature 455(7216):
1057–1060.
39. Kareiva PK, Tallis H, Ricketts TH, Daily GC, Polasky S (2011)Natural Capital: Theory &
Practice of Mapping Ecosystem Services (Oxford Univ Press, Oxford, UK).
40. Bratman GN, Hamilton JP, Daily GC (2012) The impacts of nature experience on hu-
man cognitive function and mental health.Ann N Y Acad Sci 1249(1):118–136.
41. Collins PY, et al.; Scientific Advisory Board and the Executive Committee of the Grand
Challenges on Global Mental Health (2011) Grand challenges in global mental health.
Nature 475(7354):27–30.
42. Jackson LE (2003) The relationship of urban design to human health and condition.
Landsc Urban Plan 64(4):191–200.
43. van Dillen SME, de Vries S, Groenewegen PP, Spreeuwenberg P (2012) Greenspace in
urban neighbourhoods and residents’health: Adding quality to quantity.J Epidemiol
Community Health 66(6):e8.
44. Ward Thompson C, et al. (2012) More green space is linked to less stress in deprived
communities: Evidence from salivary cortisol patterns.Landsc Urban Plan 105(3):
221–229.
45. Wu C-D, et al. (2014) Linking student performance in Massachusetts elementary
schools with the “greenness”of school surroundings using remote sensing.PLoS ONE
9(10):e108548.
46. Tzoulas K, et al. (2007) Promoting ecosystem and human health in urban areas using
Green Infrastructure: A literature review.Landsc Urban Plan 81(3):167–178.
47. Martin-Breen P, Anderies JM (2011)Resilience: A Literature Review (Rockefeller
Foundation, New York).
48. Chiesura A (2004) The role of urban parks for the sustainable city.Landsc Urban Plan
68(1):129–138.
Bratman et al.PNAS |July 14, 2015 |vol. 112 |no. 28 |8571 PSYCHOLOGICAL ANDCOGNITIVE SCIENCES
49. Schreiber F, Kavanaugh L (2014) ICLEI, 2014, Resilient Cities 2014: Congress Report.
Holzforschung 50:1–97.
50. Meyer-Lindenberg A, Tost H (2012) Neural mechanisms of social risk for psychiatric
disorders.Nat Neurosci 15(5):663–668.
51. Alsop DC, et al. (2015) Recommended implementation of arterial spin-labeled
perfusion MRI for clinical applications: A consensus of the ISMRM perfusion study
group and the European consortium for ASL in dementia.Magn Reson Med 73:
102–116.
52. Chen G, Adleman NE, Saad ZS, Leibenluft E, Cox RW (2014) Applications of multi-
variate modeling to neuroimaging group analysis: a comprehensive alternative to
univariate general linear model.Neuroimage 99:571–588.
53. Lawrence EJ, et al. (2006) The role of ‘shared representations’in social perception and
empathy: An fMRI study.Neuroimage 29(4):1173–1184.
54. Hadjikhani N, Joseph RM, Snyder J, Tager-Flusberg H (2006) Anatomical differences in
the mirror neuron system and social cognition network in autism.Cereb Cortex 16(9):
1276–1282.
55. Adolphs R (2001) The neurobiology of social cognition.Curr Opin Neurobiol 11(2):231–239.
56. Kumari V, et al. (2010) Functional MRI of verbal self-monitoring in schizophrenia:
Performance and illness-specific effects.Schizophr Bull 36(4):740–755.
57. Bigler ED, et al. (2007) Superior temporal gyrus, language function, and autism.Dev
Neuropsychol 31(2):217–238.
58. Barbey AK, Krueger F, Grafman J (2009) Structured event complexes in the medial
prefrontal cortex support counterfactual representations for future planning.Philos
Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 364(1521):1291–1300.
59. Maddock RJ, Garrett AS, Buonocore MH (2001) Remembering familiar people: the
posterior cingulate cortex and autobiographical memory retrieval.Neuroscience
104(3):667–676.
8572 |www.pnas.org/cgi/doi/10.1073/pnas.1510459112 Bratman et al.